¸ñ·Ï

Home

åǥÁö

´Ü¼øÇÔÀÇ Èû, ¿Ö ¿ì¸®´Â °£°áÇÑ ¼³¸í°ú È¿À²Àû ÇൿÀ» ¼±È£Çϴ°¡?

»ç¶÷µéÀº º»´ÉÀûÀ¸·Î °£°áÇÔÀ» ¼±È£ÇÑ´Ù. ÀÌ´Â »îÀÇ ´Ù¾çÇÑ »óȲ¿¡¼­ ¹Ýº¹ÀûÀ¸·Î È®ÀεǴ Çö»óÀ̸ç, ÃÖ±Ù ¿öÅÐ·ç ´ëÇб³(University of Waterloo)ÀÇ ÀÎÁö½É¸®ÇÐ ¿¬±¸´Â..




The Power of Simplicity, Why Do We Prefer Concise Explanations and Efficient Actions?

Simplicity Is Instinctive
People instinctively prefer simplicity. This preference is evident across countless situations in life, and recent research in cognitive psychology from the University of Waterloo has scientifically verified this intuition. Centered around the question ¡°Why are we satisfied with simple explanations?¡±, the researchers conducted seven experiments involving 2,820 participants. Consistently, the participants showed a strong preference for simple explanations and efficient action strategies.

The results reveal a fundamental principle embedded in human cognition. A simple explanation is not only easier to understand—it assumes a *trustworthy cause*. In other words, people prefer explanations based on familiar, repeatable causes over rare, complex, or unpredictable ones. This preference stems from the comfort that predictability brings when making decisions and anticipating outcomes.

Explanation and Action: The Brain Uses the Same Criteria
One of the study¡¯s most striking insights is that the human brain uses similar standards when evaluating both explanations and goal-oriented actions. The simpler the explanation and the more common the cause it contains, the more ¡°reasonable¡± it appears to people. Similarly, the simpler and more replicable the path to a goal, the more ¡°efficient¡± it is perceived to be.

This goes beyond mere information processing. It reflects what¡¯s known as *cognitive economy*—our brain¡¯s tendency to conserve mental energy while interpreting, deciding, and acting. Simplicity in explanation conserves cognitive resources; efficiency in action accelerates results. Together, these preferences form part of an evolutionary toolkit that continues to guide our behavior today.

The Core: "Simple and Reliable"
A crucial point from the study is that simplicity alone isn¡¯t enough. What people truly prefer are explanations that are both *simple and reliable*. For example, when explaining a phenomenon, participants trusted familiar, frequently occurring causes over rare or complex ones. This suggests that people don¡¯t just want the briefest explanation—they want one grounded in *predictability and reproducibility*.

The same applies to goal achievement. Rather than using intricate tools or special procedures, participants gravitated toward methods that were general and easy to implement. This preference is deeply connected to the human drive for efficiency—achieving more with fewer resources.

Implications for Organizations and Leadership
These findings extend far beyond psychology. They offer practical lessons for management, leadership, decision-making, marketing, and education. Leaders, for instance, are more persuasive when they present changes or strategies using intuitive, simple principles rather than complicated frameworks. Simple explanations build trust and activate our innate preference for actionable clarity.

Likewise, when executives define company goals and implementation plans, focusing on a few core metrics rather than complex KPIs tends to increase employee engagement and deliver better outcomes. Simplicity boosts execution, and execution drives results.

The Pitfall of Oversimplification
So, is a simple explanation always better? Not necessarily. While simplicity is a powerful preference, it can sometimes oversimplify reality and obscure critical variables. Attempting to reduce a complex phenomenon to one or two causes can lead to misinterpretation.

For example, attributing a financial crisis solely to one policy failure, or blaming an organization¡¯s dysfunction on a single leader, may feel satisfying but rarely captures the full truth. Simplicity must be bounded by *explanatory sufficiency*, and anything beyond that belongs in the realm of critical reflection.

Simplicity in AI: A Challenge for Algorithm Design
This research also holds important implications for artificial intelligence (AI). AI seeks to emulate human cognition, and the preference for simple explanations directly affects how users interact with algorithmic systems. For AI outcomes to be trusted, they must be communicated in a *clear and reliable* way, not as opaque mathematical abstractions.

As demand for Explainable AI (XAI) grows, this research offers crucial insights into how systems should be designed to enhance user trust. AI must provide explanations that are simple—but not misleading. Balancing simplicity with truthfulness is a central design challenge in the age of intelligent systems.

Lessons for Education and Communication
Simplicity is a cornerstone of effective teaching and communication. Educators are more successful when they use analogies and simple concepts instead of jargon-heavy language. Likewise, in public messaging, governments and institutions gain greater understanding and cooperation when they communicate through concise, clear messages.

However, simplicity can also be abused. Politicians or interest groups may distort truth through overly simplistic narratives—¡°The economy collapsed because of one leader¡± or ¡°This crisis was caused by outsiders.¡± These claims may sound intuitive but often lack nuance or accuracy. Simplicity should aid comprehension, not serve as a tool for manipulation.

Simplicity Is the Brain¡¯s Default Mode
As confirmed by the University of Waterloo study, humans are wired to prefer simple explanations and efficient strategies. This is both a product of evolutionary adaptation and a habit of modern reasoning. Yet simplicity should be viewed as a *means*, not an *end*. Explanations should be clear without compromising truth; actions should be efficient without losing ethical grounding.

Ultimately, simplicity is the default mode of human thinking. And how consciously and strategically we use that default may determine our intellectual resilience in an age of complexity and information overload.

(Resource - COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, November 2024, Vol. 154, ¡°Doing things efficiently: Testing an 
Account of Why Simple Explanations are Satisfying,¡± by Claudia G. Sehl, et al.  © Elsevier B.V.  all rights reserve)





´Ü¼øÇÔÀÇ Èû, ¿Ö ¿ì¸®´Â °£°áÇÑ ¼³¸í°ú È¿À²Àû ÇൿÀ» ¼±È£Çϴ°¡?

´Ü¼øÇÔÀº º»´ÉÀÌ´Ù
»ç¶÷µéÀº º»´ÉÀûÀ¸·Î °£°áÇÔÀ» ¼±È£ÇÑ´Ù. ÀÌ´Â »îÀÇ ´Ù¾çÇÑ »óȲ¿¡¼­ ¹Ýº¹ÀûÀ¸·Î È®ÀεǴ Çö»óÀ̸ç, ÃÖ±Ù ¿öÅÐ·ç ´ëÇб³(University of Waterloo)ÀÇ ÀÎÁö½É¸®ÇÐ ¿¬±¸´Â ÀÌ Á÷°üÀ» °úÇÐÀûÀ¸·Î ÀÔÁõÇß´Ù. ¿¬±¸ÁøÀº ¡°¿ì¸®´Â ¿Ö ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸í¿¡ ¸¸Á·Çϴ°¡?¡±¶ó´Â Áú¹®À» Áß½ÉÀ¸·Î 7°³ÀÇ ½ÇÇèÀ» ÁøÇàÇß°í, 2,820¸íÀÇ Âü°¡ÀÚµéÀº ¿¹¿Ü ¾øÀÌ ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸í°ú È¿À²ÀûÀÎ ½ÇÇà Àü·«À» ¼±È£ÇÏ´Â °æÇâÀ» º¸¿´´Ù.

ÀÌ ¿¬±¸ °á°ú´Â Àΰ£ÀÇ »ç°í ü°è¿¡ ³»ÀçµÈ ÀϰüµÈ ¿ø¸®¸¦ µå·¯³½´Ù. ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸íÀº ´ÜÁö ÀÌÇØÇϱ⠽¬¿î °ÍÀÌ ¾Æ´Ï¶ó, ¡®½Å·ÚÇÒ ¼ö ÀÖ´Â ¿øÀΡ¯À» ÀüÁ¦·Î ÇÑ´Ù. ´Ù½Ã ¸»ÇØ, »ç¶÷µéÀº µå¹°°í º¹ÀâÇÏ¸ç ¿¹Ãø ºÒ°¡´ÉÇÑ ¿øÀκ¸´Ù´Â, Àͼ÷ÇÏ°í ¹Ýº¹ °¡´ÉÇÑ ¿øÀÎÀ» ¼³¸íÀÇ ±âÁØÀ¸·Î ¼±È£ÇÑ´Ù. ÀÌ´Â °á°ú¸¦ ¿¹ÃøÇϰí ÇൿÀ» °èȹÇÒ ¶§ ¾ÈÁ¤°¨À» Á¦°øÇϱ⠶§¹®ÀÌ´Ù.

¼³¸í°ú Çൿ, ³ú´Â °°Àº ±¸Á¶·Î ÆÇ´ÜÇÑ´Ù
¿¬±¸ÀÇ ÇÙ½É ÅëÂû Áß Çϳª´Â, »ç¶÷µéÀÌ ¼³¸íÀ» ¼±ÅÃÇÒ ¶§¿Í ¸ñÇ¥ ´Þ¼º ¼ö´ÜÀ» ¼±ÅÃÇÒ ¶§ ³ú°¡ À¯»çÇÑ ±âÁØÀ» Àû¿ëÇÑ´Ù´Â Á¡ÀÌ´Ù. ¼³¸íÀÌ °£°áÇÒ¼ö·Ï, ±×¸®°í ±× ¼³¸íÀÌ ÈçÇÑ ¿øÀÎÀ» Æ÷ÇÔÇÒ¼ö·Ï »ç¶÷µéÀº ±×°ÍÀ» ¡®ÇÕ¸®Àû¡¯À̶ó°í ´À²¼´Ù. µ¿ÀÏÇϰÔ, ¾î¶² ¸ñÇ¥¸¦ ´Þ¼ºÇϱâ À§ÇÑ ¹æ¹ýÀÌ °£´ÜÇϰí ÀçÇö °¡´ÉÇÒ¼ö·Ï, »ç¶÷µéÀº ±×°ÍÀ» ¡®È¿À²Àû¡¯À̶ó°í Æò°¡Çß´Ù.

ÀÌ´Â ´Ü¼øÈ÷ Á¤º¸ ó¸®ÀÇ ¹®Á¦¸¦ ³Ñ¾î, ÀÎÁö °æÁ¦¼º(cognitive economy)°ú °ü·ÃµÇ¾î ÀÖ´Ù. Àΰ£ÀÇ ³ú´Â ¿¡³ÊÁö¸¦ Àû°Ô µéÀÌ°íµµ ¸¹Àº °ÍÀ» ÇØ¼®Çϰí, °áÁ¤Çϰí, ÇൿÇϱâ À§ÇØ ¼³°èµÇ¾î ÀÖ´Ù. ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸íÀº ¿¡³ÊÁö Àý¾àÀÇ Áö¸§±æÀ̰í, È¿À²ÀûÀÎ ÇൿÀº °á°ú¸¦ ºü¸£°Ô ¾ò´Â Àü·«ÀÌ´Ù. ÀÌ µÎ °¡Áö´Â Àΰ£ÀÌ È¯°æ¿¡ ÀûÀÀÇØ ¿Â ÁøÈ­Àû »ê¹°À̸ç, Çö´ë »çȸ¿¡¼­µµ ¿©ÀüÈ÷ À¯È¿ÇÑ ÆÇ´Ü ±âÁØÀ¸·Î ÀÛµ¿ÇÑ´Ù.

¡°´Ü¼øÇÏÁö¸¸ ½Å·ÚÇÒ ¼ö Àִ¡±ÀÌ ÇÙ½É
À̹ø ¿¬±¸¿¡¼­ ƯÈ÷ ÁÖ¸ñÇÒ Á¡Àº, ´Ü¼øÇÔ¸¸À¸·Î´Â ºÎÁ·ÇÏ´Ù´Â °ÍÀÌ´Ù. »ç¶÷µéÀº ¡®´Ü¼øÇϸ鼭µµ ½Å·ÚÇÒ ¼ö ÀÖ´Â(common and reliable)¡¯ ¿øÀÎÀ» ¼±È£Çß´Ù. ¿¹¸¦ µé¾î, ¾î¶² Çö»óÀÇ ¿øÀÎÀ» ¼³¸íÇÒ ¶§, »ç¶÷µéÀº µå¹°°í º¹ÀâÇÑ ¿øÀκ¸´Ù ÈçÇÏ°í ¹Ýº¹ÀûÀ¸·Î ³ªÅ¸³ª´Â ¿øÀÎÀ» ´õ ½Å·ÚÇß´Ù. ÀÌ´Â Àΰ£ÀÇ »ç°í°¡ ´Ü¼øÈ÷ ÃÖ¼ÒÇÑÀÇ ¼³¸íÀ» ¼±È£ÇÏ´Â °ÍÀÌ ¾Æ´Ï¶ó, **¿¹Ãø °¡´É¼º°ú ÀçÇö¼º**À̶ó´Â ½Å·ÚÀÇ ±âÃʸ¦ ¿ä±¸ÇÑ´Ù´Â °ÍÀ» º¸¿©ÁØ´Ù.

ÀÌ·¯ÇÑ ¼ºÇâÀº ¼³¸í»Ó¸¸ ¾Æ´Ï¶ó ¸ñÇ¥ ´Þ¼º Àü·«¿¡¼­µµ µ¿ÀÏÇÏ°Ô ³ªÅ¸³µ´Ù. º¹ÀâÇÏ°í Æ¯¼öÇÑ µµ±¸³ª ÀýÂ÷º¸´Ù, ÈçÇϰí ÀϹÝÈ­µÈ ¹æ¹ýÀÌ »ç¶÷µé¿¡°Ô ´õ ¼±È£µÇ¾ú´Ù. ÀÌ´Â ¡°ÀûÀº ÀÚ¿øÀ¸·Î ´õ ¸¹Àº °á°ú¸¦ ¾ò°íÀÚ Çϴ¡± Àΰ£ÀÇ È¿À² Ãß±¸ º»´É°ú ±íÀÌ ¿¬°áµÇ¾î ÀÖ´Ù.

Á¶Á÷°ú ¸®´õ½Ê¿¡ ÁÖ´Â ÇÔÀÇ
ÀÌ ¿¬±¸ °á°ú´Â ´ÜÁö ½É¸®ÇÐÀû ¹ß°ß¿¡ ±×Ä¡Áö ¾Ê´Â´Ù. ½Ç¹«ÀûÀ¸·Î´Â Á¶Á÷°ü¸®, ¸®´õ½Ê, ÀÇ»ç°áÁ¤, ¸¶ÄÉÆÃ, ±³À° µî ´Ù¾çÇÑ ºÐ¾ß¿¡ Àû¿ë °¡´ÉÇÏ´Ù. ¿¹¸¦ µé¾î, ¸®´õ´Â ±¸¼º¿ø¿¡°Ô º¯È­³ª Àü·«À» ¼³¸íÇÒ ¶§, º¹ÀâÇÑ ÇÁ·¹ÀÓ¿öÅ©º¸´Ù Á÷°üÀûÀÌ°í ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¿ø¸®¸¦ Á¦½ÃÇÒ ¶§ ¼³µæ·ÂÀÌ ³ô´Ù. ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸íÀÌ ½Å·Ú¸¦ À¯µµÇϰí, È¿À²ÀûÀÎ ½ÇÇà ¹æ½ÄÀ» ¼±È£ÇÏ´Â Àΰ£ÀÇ º»¼ºÀ» ÀÚ±ØÇϱ⠶§¹®ÀÌ´Ù.

¶ÇÇÑ, °æ¿µÁøÀÌ Á¶Á÷ÀÇ ¸ñÇ¥¸¦ ¼ö¸³ÇÏ°í ½ÇÇà Àü·«À» Á¦½ÃÇÒ ¶§, º¹ÀâÇÑ KPI ü°èº¸´Ù´Â ÇÙ½ÉÀûÀÎ ÁöÇ¥ ¸î °¡Áö¿¡ ÁýÁßÇÏ´Â °ÍÀÌ Á÷¿øµéÀÇ ¸ôÀÔµµ¸¦ ³ôÀÌ°í ½ÇÇà °¡´É¼ºÀ» ³ôÀδÙ. ÀÌ´Â ´Ü¼øÇÔÀÌ °ð ½ÇÇà·ÂÀ̸ç, ½ÇÇà·ÂÀÌ °ð ¼º°ú¶ó´Â °ø½ÄÀ» º¸¿©ÁÖ´Â »ç·Ê´Ù.

´Ü¼øÇÔÀÇ ÇÔÁ¤: ¾ðÁ¦³ª È¿°úÀûÀΰ¡?
±×·¸´Ù¸é ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸íÀÌ Ç×»ó ´õ ³ªÀº°¡? ±×·¸Áö´Â ¾Ê´Ù. ´Ü¼øÇÔÀÇ ¼±È£´Â È®½ÇÈ÷ °­·ÂÇÑ °æÇ⼺ÀÌÁö¸¸, ¶§·Î´Â Çö½ÇÀ» °úµµÇÏ°Ô Ãà¼ÒÇϰųª Áß¿äÇÑ º¯¼ö¸¦ °£°úÇÒ À§ÇèÀÌ ÀÖ´Ù. º¹ÀâÇÑ Çö»óÀ» Áö³ªÄ¡°Ô °£·«ÇÏ°Ô ¼³¸íÇÏ·Á Çϸé, ¡®°úµµÇÑ ´Ü¼øÈ­(oversimplification)¡¯¶ó´Â ¿À·ù¿¡ ºüÁú ¼ö ÀÖ´Ù.

¿¹ÄÁ´ë, °æÁ¦ À§±âÀÇ ¿øÀÎÀ» ´Ü ÇϳªÀÇ Á¤Ã¥ ½ÇÆÐ·Î ȯ¿øÇѴٰųª, Á¶Á÷ ¹®Á¦¸¦ ÇÑ ¸íÀÇ ¸®´õ½Ê ½ÇÆÐ·Î ¼³¸íÇÏ´Â ¹æ½ÄÀº ¸Å·ÂÀûÀÏ ¼ö´Â À־ Áø½Ç¿¡ °¡±î¿î ¼³¸íÀº ¾Æ´Ò ¼ö ÀÖ´Ù. µû¶ó¼­ ´Ü¼øÇÔÀº ¡®ÃæºÐÇÑ ¼³¸í·Â¡¯À» ÃæÁ·ÇÏ´Â ¼±¿¡¼­ À¯ÁöµÇ¾î¾ß Çϸç, ±× ¹Ù±ùÀº ¼ºÂûÀÇ ¿µ¿ªÀ¸·Î ³²°Ü¾ß ÇÑ´Ù.

ÀΰøÁö´É°ú ´Ü¼øÇÔ: ¾Ë°í¸®Áò ¼³°èÀÇ °úÁ¦
ÀÌ ¿¬±¸´Â ÀΰøÁö´É(AI) ¼³°è¿¡µµ Áß¿äÇÑ ÇÔÀǸ¦ ÁØ´Ù. AI´Â Àΰ£ÀÇ ÀÎÁö ÆÐÅÏÀ» ¸ð¹æÇÏ·Á´Â ±â¼úÀ̸ç, »ç¶÷µéÀÇ ¡®´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸í¿¡ ´ëÇÑ ¼±È£¡¯´Â ¾Ë°í¸®ÁòÀÇ Ãâ·Â ¹æ½Ä¿¡µµ ¿µÇâÀ» ¹ÌÄ¥ ¼ö ÀÖ´Ù. »ç¿ëÀÚ°¡ AIÀÇ ÆÇ´ÜÀ» ½Å·ÚÇÏ·Á¸é, ±× °á°ú°¡ º¹ÀâÇÑ ¼öÇÐÀû °è»êÀÌ ¾Æ´Ñ **°£°áÇÏ°í ½Å·Ú °¡´ÉÇÑ ¹æ½Ä**À¸·Î ¼³¸íµÇ¾î¾ß ÇÑ´Ù.

ÀÌ¹Ì ¡®¼³¸í °¡´ÉÇÑ AI(Explainable AI, XAI)¡¯¿¡ ´ëÇÑ ¿ä±¸°¡ Áõ°¡Çϰí ÀÖ´Â °¡¿îµ¥, À̹ø ¿¬±¸´Â »ç¿ëÀÚÀÇ ½É¸®Àû ¼ö¿ë¼ºÀ» °í·ÁÇÑ ÀÎÅÍÆäÀ̽º ¼³°è¿¡ Áß¿äÇÑ ÅëÂûÀ» Á¦°øÇÑ´Ù. AI´Â ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸íÀ» Á¦°øÇØ¾ß ÇÏÁö¸¸, µ¿½Ã¿¡ Á¤º¸ÀÇ ¿Ö°îÀ̳ª °úÀ× ´Ü¼øÈ­´Â ÇÇÇØ¾ß ÇÑ´Ù´Â ÀÌÁß °úÁ¦¸¦ ¾È°í ÀÖ´Ù.

±³À°°ú Ä¿¹Â´ÏÄÉÀ̼ǿ¡ ÁÖ´Â ±³ÈÆ
´Ü¼øÇÔÀº ±³À°°ú Ä¿¹Â´ÏÄÉÀ̼ÇÀÇ ÇÙ½É ¿øÄ¢À̱⵵ ÇÏ´Ù. ±³»ç´Â Çлý¿¡°Ô »õ·Î¿î °³³äÀ» ¼³¸íÇÒ ¶§, º¹ÀâÇÑ Àü¹® ¿ë¾îº¸´Ù °£°áÇϰí À¯ºñÀûÀÎ ¼³¸íÀ» »ç¿ëÇÒ ¶§ ÇнÀ È¿°ú°¡ ³ô´Ù. ¸¶Âù°¡Áö·Î, °ø°ø Ä¿¹Â´ÏÄÉÀ̼ǿ¡¼­µµ ±¹¹ÎÀ» ¼³µæÇϰí ÀÌÇØ½ÃŰ·Á¸é ª°í ¸í·áÇÑ ¸Þ½ÃÁö°¡ È¿°úÀûÀÌ´Ù.

±×·¯³ª ¿©±â¼­µµ ´Ü¼øÇÔÀÇ À§ÇèÀº Á¸ÀçÇÑ´Ù. ¡®´Ü¼øÇÑ ¸Þ½ÃÁö¡¯´Â ¶§·Î´Â ¡®Á¤Ä¡Àû ¿Ö°î¡¯À¸·Î ¾Ç¿ëµÇ±â ½±±â ¶§¹®ÀÌ´Ù. ¿¹ÄÁ´ë ¡°°æÁ¦´Â Á¤ºÎ°¡ ¸ÁÃÆ´Ù¡± ȤÀº ¡°¹®Á¦´Â ¿ÜºÎ ¼¼·Â ¶§¹®ÀÌ´Ù¡± °°Àº ÁÖÀåÀº ¼³¸íÀ¸·Î¼­´Â ´Ü¼øÇÏÁö¸¸, ±× ÀÚü°¡ »ç½ÇÀÌ ¾Æ´Ò ¼ö ÀÖ´Ù. µû¶ó¼­ ´Ü¼øÇÔÀº Àü´Þ ¹æ½ÄÀÌ µÇ¾î¾ß ÇÏÁö, Áø½ÇÀÇ ¿Ö°î ¼ö´ÜÀÌ µÇ¾î¼­´Â ¾È µÈ´Ù.

´Ü¼øÇÔÀº »ç°íÀÇ ±âº»°ªÀÌ´Ù
¿öÅÐ·ç ´ëÇб³ ¿¬±¸ÆÀÀÌ È®ÀÎÇÑ ¹Ù¿Í °°ÀÌ, Àΰ£Àº ´Ü¼øÇÑ ¼³¸í°ú È¿À²ÀûÀÎ ½ÇÇàÀ» º»´ÉÀûÀ¸·Î ¼±È£ÇÑ´Ù. ÀÌ´Â ÁøÈ­Àû »ýÁ¸ Àü·«ÀÌÀÚ, Çö´ë »çȸ¿¡¼­µµ À¯È¿ÇÑ »ç°í ½À°üÀÌ´Ù. ±×·¯³ª ´Ü¼øÇÔÀº ¸ñÇ¥°¡ ¾Æ´Ï¶ó ¼ö´ÜÀ̾î¾ß ÇÑ´Ù. ¼³¸íÀº ¸íÈ®Ç쵂 Áø½ÇÀ» ³õÄ¡Áö ¾Ê¾Æ¾ß Çϰí, ÇൿÀº È¿À²ÀûÀÌµÇ À±¸®¼ºÀ» ÀҾ´Â ¾È µÈ´Ù.

±Ã±ØÀûÀ¸·Î ´Ü¼øÇÔÀº Àΰ£ »ç°íÀÇ ±âº»°ª(default)ÀÌ´Ù. ±×¸®°í ¿ì¸®°¡ ÀÌ ±âº»°ªÀ» ¾ó¸¶³ª ÀǽÄÀûÀ¸·Î, Àü·«ÀûÀ¸·Î Ȱ¿ëÇÏ´ÂÁö°¡ Á¤º¸ °úÀ× ½Ã´ë¸¦ »ì¾Æ°¡´Â ÁöÀû ŵµÀÇ ¼öÁØÀ» °áÁ¤ÇÏ°Ô µÉ °ÍÀÌ´Ù.

(Ãâó - COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, November 2024, Vol. 154, ¡°Doing things efficiently: Testing an 
Account of Why Simple Explanations are Satisfying,¡± by Claudia G. Sehl, et al.  © Elsevier B.V.  all rights reserve)